Posted: February 27th, 2023

Power point

 Option 1:Option 2:You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your presentation and add a voice-over.

  • If you are unable to add voice-over to your PowerPoint,  you will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your presentation.
  • Next, you will use Screencast-o-Matic (or a similar program) to create a video recording of your screen and voice as your present the information.
  • Third, you will upload the video presentation to YouTube so your professor can view it. If you choose this option, you will submit your articles as well as the PowerPoint (or equivalent) file and the link to the YouTube presentation to complete this assignment. 

Guidelines:

  1. The presentation must include both audio (your voice explaining the information) and visual (PowerPoint presentation including text and/or images). Videos should not be used within the presentation.
  2. The presentation should include the following three aspects: 

    An overview of your specific topic and its importance and application in current society. 
    Theory related to your topic (please refer to Chapters 1 and 2 for theories related to Human Development) 
    Discussion of development in at least three stages (e.g., infancy, early childhood, middle childhood, adolescence, emerging adulthood, young adults, middle age, elderly) unless written permission was given by your instruction to instead do a deeper evaluation of a particular topic during one developmental period.  

  3. The presentation must be 15 minutes long (no more than 20).
  4. The presentation must include information from at least 5 scholarly sources (if used, the course textbook does not count as one of these 5 sources). At least 3 of these should include original research reports typically contain sections with subtitles such as “Abstract”, “Introduction”, “Methods”, “Results”, “Discussion” and “References”. 
  5. APA style citations should be used within the presentation. A reference section (in APA style) should appear at the end of the presentation.

Resources:

  • If you do not have PowerPoint, Google Slides is a free alternative.
  • You may need a headset with a microphone to record your presentation if your computer does not have a built-in microphone.
  • Click here for a brief introduction to using Screencast-o-Matic. The program will create a video file that can be saved on your computer and shared via YouTube.
  • PowerPoint 101: Everything You Need to Make a Basic Presentation
  • 10 Tips for More Effective PowerPoint Presentations
  • 10 Tips for a Good Presentation 
  • My topic is Bilingual Aphasia . please add pictures and notes to power point. The first article that was chosen in week one is attached.

  • Title Page
  • Does the Thematic Hierarchy hold in people with aphasia and across the lifespan?
    Evidence from the Event Task.

    by

    Sophia Norvilas

    Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of the

    Dietrich School of Arts and Sciences and

    Fredrick Honors College in partial fulfillment

    of the requirements for the degree of

    Bachelor of Philosophy

    University of Pittsburgh

    2022

    ii

    Commit tee Membership Page

    UNIVERSITY OF PITTSBURGH

    DIETRICH SCHOOL OF ARTS AND SCIENCES

    This thesis was presented

    by

    Sophia Norvilas

    It was defended on

    November 11,

    2022

    and approved by

    Dr. Tessa Warren, Professor, Psychology

    Dr. Julie Fiez, Professor, Psychology

    Dr. Jamie Reilly, Professor, Communication Science and Disorders

    Thesis Advisor: Dr. Michael Walsh Dickey, Professor, Communication Science and Disorders

    iii

    Copyright © by Sophia Norvilas

    2022

    iv

  • Abstract
  • Does the Thematic Hierarchy hold in people with aphasia and across the lifespan?
    Evidence from the Event Task.

    Sophia Norvilas

    University of Pittsburgh, 2022

    Aphasia is a neurological disorder that can disrupt language production and

    comprehension, impairing both written and spoken language (Dresang et al., 2019). This condition

    is typically brought on by brain damage following a stroke. Research indicates that people with

    aphasia sometimes rely on event knowledge to compensate for their language impairment

    (Caramazza & Zurif, 1976). However, we know little about event processing in people with

    aphasia (PWA), even though event knowledge supports a multitude of crucial cognitive processes,

    including language comprehension, language production, memory, and perception. One type of

    event knowledge that has been studied thoroughly in linguistics is the entities, objects, and

    locations (event roles) that are involved in events. Linguists have developed a hypothesis, known

    as the Thematic Hierarchy, that some of these event roles are more cognitively salient than others.

    The research I present here uses evidence from a new assessment that measures event knowledge

    (the Event Task) to evaluate whether this thematic knowledge is maintained in older adults and

    people with aphasia, while also examining whether the performance of PWA on the Event Task is

    aligned with the Thematic Hierarchy. PWA (N = 26) and neurologically healthy adults (N = 182)

    completed the Event Task, which instructed participants to identify whether a depicted event was

    plausible or implausible. Analyses showed that the Thematic Hierarchy did not appear to guide

    the performance of PWA or neurologically healthy adults across the lifespan. However, PWA and

    neurologically healthy controls alike displayed the same patterns of both accuracy and reaction

    v

    time performance. In neurologically healthy adults, increased age was correlated with decreased

    accuracy and increased reaction times. In addition, neurologically healthy adults with 12+ years

    of education were found to have increased accuracy and decreased reaction times. The current

    findings could be the foundation for future research regarding aphasia and event knowledge.

    vi

  • Table of Contents
  • Preface
  • ………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….. x

  • 1.0 Introduction
  • …………………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 1

    1.1 Semantic memory and event knowledge ……………………………………………………………. 1

    1.2 The Thematic Hierarchy …………………………………………………………………………………… 2

    1.3 The Event Task ………………………………………………………………………………………………… 5

    1.4 The Current Study …………………………………………………………………………………………… 7

  • 2.0 Methods
  • …………………………………………………………………………………………………………………… 8

    2.1 Participants ……………………………………………………………………………………………………… 8

    2.2 Event Task Stimuli and Procedure …………………………………………………………………….

    8

    2.3 Data Compilation ……………………………………………………………………………………………… 9

    2.4 Event Taxonomy Questionnaire ………………………………………………………………………. 10

    2.5 Data Analysis …………………………………………………………………………………………………. 11

  • 3.0 Results
  • …………………………………………………………………………………………………………………… 12

    3.1 PWA and Matched Controls Analyses ……………………………………………………………..

    12

    3.2 PWA Analyses ………………………………………………………………………………………………… 14

    3.3 Lifespan Analyses ……………………………………………………………………………………………

    17

    3.3.1 Age Analyses ………………………………………………………………………………………….18

    3.3.2 Mini-Mental State Examination Analyses ……………………………………………….20

    3.3.3 Years of Education Analyses …………………………………………………………………..22

  • 4.0 Discussion
  • ………………………………………………………………………………………………………………. 24

    4.1 PWA and Matched Controls …………………………………………………………………………… 25

    vii

    4.2 Neurologically Healthy Adults Across the Lifespan …………………………………………..

    27

    4.3 Clinical Application ………………………………………………………………………………………… 30

    4.4 Limitations …………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 34

  • 5.0 Conclusion
  • …………………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 35

  • Bibliography
  • ……………………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 37

    viii

  • List of Tables
  • Table 1: Canonical and Exclusive Event Role Violations ………………………………………………. 11

    ix

  • List of Figures
  • Figure 1: PWA and matched control participants’ accuracy performance. …………………….

    13

    Figure 2: PWA and matched control participants’ reaction time performance. ………………

    14

    Figure 3: Individual PWA accuracy performance per event role violation type. ……………..

    16

    Figure 4: Individual PWA reaction time performance per event role violation type ………. 17

    Figure 5: Healthy participants’ accuracy performance in relation to age. ………………………

    18

    Figure 6: Healthy participants’ reaction time performance in relation to age. ………………..

    19

    Figure 7: Healthy participants’ accuracy performance in relation to MMSE Score. ……….

    20

    Figure 8: Healthy participants’ reaction time performance in relation to MMSE Score. …

    21

    Figure 9: Healthy participants’ accuracy performance in relation to years of education. ..

    22

    Figure 10: Healthy participants’ reaction time performance in relation to years of education.

    …………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………. 23

    x

    Preface

    First and foremost, I would like to express my gratitude to Dr. Michael Walsh Dickey, my

    mentor and Bachelor of Philosophy advisor. Thank you for the years of guidance, encouragement,

    and insight. I will never forget the invaluable lessons he taught me and the countless hours he

    dedicated to my project. Joining the Language and Brain Lab and learning under his guidance truly

    changed the trajectory of my career, and I cannot express the full extent of my appreciation. In

    addition, I am so lucky to have studied under Dr. Tessa Warren, my incredible Honors in

    Psychology advisor. Her mentorship has been imperative in honing my writing skills and finding

    my voice in the scientific community. Her dedication and expertise have been instrumental in

    helping me become the researcher I am today.

    I am very grateful to Dr. Julie Fiez and Dr. Jamie Reilly, for their enthusiasm and

    willingness to serve on my thesis committee. I would like to acknowledge the University of

    Pittsburgh Honors College for providing this wonderful opportunity and supporting my work with

    resources such as the Brackenridge Fellowship. I am extremely appreciative of Emily Goldberg,

    for assisting with data compilation efforts and cheering me on since my very first lab presentation

    over two years ago. Finally, I would like to thank my friends and family, for providing unwavering

    support and encouragement as I pursued this project.

    1

    1.0 Introduction

    1.1 Semantic memory and event knowledge

    Semantic memory is an individual’s long-lasting memory for general knowledge about the

    world (Dresang et al., 2019). From birth, we begin collecting knowledge about the world. Semantic

    memory is critical when trying to produce or understand language. For example, when you hear a

    word or sentence, you consult your knowledge about objects, people, actions, and things to

    understand the meaning of that sentence. Semantic memory is often impaired in PWA, and an

    accurate measure of semantic memory impairment is crucial for treatment (Antonucci & Reilly,

    2008). Existing picture-based assessments of object-related semantic memory include Pyramids

    and Palm Trees (PPT) (Howard et al., 1992) and the Camels and Cactus Test (CCT) (Bozeat et al.,

    2000). There is also a picture-based assessment of action-related semantic memory called the

    Kissing and Dancing Test (KDT) (Bak & Hodges, 2003). However, there are few tests to measure

    semantic memory for events in PWA (Dresang et al., 2019).

    Events can be defined as collections of people, objects, actions, places, and the context in

    which they occur (Dresang et al, 2019). Event roles (or thematic roles) are the people, places, and

    things that play a role in an event. The agent is the initiator of an action (Ünal et al., 2021). The

    patient is the entity undergoing the effect of some action, often undergoing some change in state

    (Ünal et al., 2021). The instrument is the tool used to perform an action (Ünal et al., 2021). The

    location is the place in which something is situated (Saeed et. al, 1996). The goal is the entity

    towards which something moves, either literally or metaphorically (Ünal et al., 2021). An example

    of this would be the following sentence: “The boy hit the baseball with his bat on the field.” The

    2

    agent would be “the boy” as he is the one doing the act. The action is hit. The patient is the baseball

    because it is moved by the action. The instrument is the bat because the boy uses the bat as a tool

    to hit the ball. The location is the baseball field on which he is playing the game.

    Event role knowledge is activated during language processing. Some evidence for this

    comes from a study in which adult participants were given line drawings of two-participant

    events—for example, a man throwing a football to a woman—and were asked to describe the

    events (Griffin & Bock, 2000). Eye-tracking data showed that the participants’ eye movements

    were guided by event features prior to sentence formulation. Participants gazed at the agent prior

    to speech, then looked to the patient as they began speaking, and finally returned their gaze to the

    agent. The results showed that participants “rapidly extract roles of event participants” while

    describing the line drawing (Griffin & Bock, 2000). Event knowledge provides context that helps

    individuals understand novel events by providing prototypical events for comparison (McRae &

    Matsuki, 2009). Language processing is reliant on event knowledge, as it supports verb retrieval

    and thematic-role processing (Dresang et al., 2019).

    1.2 The Thematic Hierarchy

    The Thematic Hierarchy is the idea that certain event roles are more prominent than others

    (Ünal et al., 2021). An event role near the top of the hierarchy would have more prominence,

    meaning that it should be identified earlier and processed more quickly than a non-prominent event

    role. Agents are at the top of the hierarchy and are most salient or prominent. Next are Patients,

    followed by Goals. Instruments are second-to-lowest on the thematic hierarchy. Finally, locations

    are the least prominent role. Linguistic research indicates that agents are most prominent because

    3

    they are linguistically encoded as subjects (Baker, 1997; Jackendoff, 1990). Patients are next

    because they are direct objects of the verb. Goals, instruments, and locations are the least likely to

    be in these syntactic positions, leading to a lower prominence on the thematic hierarchy.

    Instruments are rarely selected as verb arguments and even referred to as “secondary roles” (Baker,

    1997). This hierarchy naturally predicts that event roles towards the top of the Thematic Hierarchy

    will be mentioned more frequently or identified more easily than event roles from the bottom of

    the Thematic

    Hierarchy.

    I will now outline the experimental evidence supporting the Thematic Hierarchy. Agents

    and patients are expected to be prioritized over other event roles because of their positions towards

    the top of the Thematic Hierarchy. Consistent with this, Hafri et al (2018) found that information

    about agents and patients is often extracted even when it is not directly encouraged by a task or

    while the participant is preoccupied with another task (Hafri et al., 2018). Participants viewed an

    array of stimuli images depicting a simple interaction between a male and a female and were

    instructed to answer which character, the male or the female, was on the left or right half of the

    screen. When presented with subsequent images in which the event role of the target character

    differed from one trial to the next, the participants displayed an event role switch cost, a delay in

    reaction time caused by the shift in event role. Hafri et al.’s study found that although task

    instructions did not explicitly refer to event roles, participants’ reaction times were affected by the

    event role change, indicating that event role information is spontaneously encoded even when not

    directly related to the task at hand.

    Research suggests that instruments are not prioritized unless the instrument is deemed

    particularly relevant to the story being told. Studies show that English speaking adults consistently

    omit instruments in retelling stories involving instrument events (Lockridge & Brennan, 2002).

    4

    The instrument is even more likely to be omitted from the retelling if it is a common or typical

    instrument. Lockridge and Brennan (2002) found that during story retellings, atypical instruments

    were most often explicitly disclosed to the partner early in the story, whereas an instrument that is

    inferable from the story was not mentioned as frequently. These findings suggest that although

    instruments can provide crucial information about an event, they are not attended to with the same

    focus that an agent or patient would receive, which is consistent with their low role on the Thematic

    Hierarchy.

    Location

    s are positioned at the bottom of the Thematic Hierarchy. Ferretti et al (2001) used

    single word priming to investigate how verbs activate event role knowledge (Ferretti et al., 2001).

    Results showed that verbs primed all event roles except for locations (Ferretti et al., 2001). These

    findings are consistent with the idea that locations are the least salient of all event roles, resulting

    in location’s place at the bottom of the Thematic Hierarchy.

    From a cognitive perspective, the Thematic Hierarchy would predict that entities in more

    prominent event roles would be remembered better and identified more easily than entities in less

    prominent roles. This claim is supported by a study by Ünal et al. (2021) that tested young learners

    of English and Turkish in order to determine whether asymmetries in event role prominence

    generalize to other languages. The study had two parts: a linguistic description task and a change-

    blindness task. In the linguistic description task, participants were asked to describe event stimuli

    images. Event role prioritization was measured by how frequently participants mentioned each

    event role when describing a stimuli image. For the change-blindness task, four new stimuli images

    were created from each original stimuli image by changing the color of one event role. In the task,

    the original stimuli image was displayed, followed by a grey screen. Next, the new stimuli image

    featuring the color change was shown, followed by another grey screen. The cycle would then

    5

    repeat. Participants were instructed to respond as soon as they detected the changing object. In this

    task, event role prioritization was measured by how quickly the participant responded to the

    changed event role.

    Findings suggested that agents were prioritized in both language groups. Patients and goals

    tended to be similarly prioritized over instruments in both languages as well. The relative salience

    of event roles was similar across young learners of both English and Turkish (Ünal et

    al., 2021).

    This is particularly interesting because even though there is variation in the way these event roles

    are encoded across the two languages, event roles were prioritized in similar ways, which aligns

    with the Thematic Hierarchy. My thesis explores the theory that people should be faster and more

    accurate to detect incongruency when it appears in more prominent roles than in less prominent

    roles.

    1.3 The Event Task

    The Event Task is a picture-based assessment of semantic memory. The preliminary

    version of the assessment was developed in the Language and Brain Lab at the University of

    Pittsburgh. The task’s goal is to measure how functional or impaired an individual’s ability to

    access their semantic memory for events is. In the task, the participant is given photographs of

    events that are either plausible or implausible. Plausible events would be those that would

    commonly occur in everyday life, such as a person doing work at a desk or boxing in a gym.

    Implausible events are impossible or unlikely, and do not match our everyday experience or our

    world knowledge based in semantic memory. Images like a man playing the violin underwater and

    a woman with her head in a bag are clear examples of implausible events taken directly from the

    6

    Event stimuli. In each implausible image, at least one event role is incongruent with the event

    depicted. Participants were shown one image at a time and were asked to indicate if this scene was

    something that may normally happen.

    A study conducted by Dresang et al. (2019) assessed the preliminary validity of the Event

    Task. The study’s first aim was to characterize typical performance and distinguish stimulus

    characteristics of the Event Task. To that end, it gathered data from 90 neurologically healthy

    adults across the lifespan and identified both average performance at multiple age-ranges and

    variation in processing time across the plausible and implausible images (Dresang et al., 2019).

    The second aim was to establish the Event Task’s sensitivity to neurological impairment. Findings

    concluded that the Event Task was successful in detecting those participants afflicted with aphasia,

    indicated by distinctions between neurologically healthy age-matched controls and PWA (Dresang

    et al., 2019). The third and fourth aims were to demonstrate that the Event Task shows preliminary

    evidence of convergent validity with measures of language processing and assessments of action

    and object knowledge. Poor Event Task performance was correlated with greater deficits in verb

    retrieval (specifically, verb production) and thematic-role processing (specifically, producing

    agent, patient, and other thematic roles in sentences), and Event Task performance was predicted

    by KDT (action based semantic memory assessment) performance and PPT (object based semantic

    memory assessment) performance (Dresang et al., 2019). These findings suggest that Event Task

    performance is indicative of verb retrieval abilities and thematic-role processing abilities, which

    are crucial for language processing. The current study expands upon the foundational work of

    Dresang et al. by examining whether the Thematic Hierarchy’s predictions regarding the salience

    of various event roles hold for Dresang et al.’s Event Task data.

    7

    1.4 The Current Study

    My research investigated performance on the Event Task in relation to the Thematic

    Hierarchy. My research tested whether there was a correlation between the relative prominence of

    event roles according to the Thematic Hierarchy (agents > patients > instruments > locations) and

    accuracy and reaction time for implausible Event Task pictures which contain violations of our

    expectations about those event roles. For example, the stimulus image of a man playing the violin

    in the ocean would be a location violation, as the location (the ocean) is not a typical place for that

    event to occur. I hypothesized that there would be a positive correlation between Thematic

    Hierarchy position and accuracy, with higher accuracy for more prominent event roles. I

    hypothesized that there would be a negative correlation between Thematic Hierarchy position and

    reaction time, with lower (or faster) reaction times for more prominent event roles.

    If the event roles identified as incongruent most accurately and quickly were those event

    roles near the top of the Thematic Hierarchy, that would suggest the Thematic Hierarchy is a good

    indicator of event role prominence. If the Thematic Hierarchy proves to be a good indicator of

    which event roles elicit the fastest or most accurate responses in the Event Task, then it might

    suggest that aphasia treatment could be adjusted to focus on improving comprehension of those

    event role categories that PWA respond to inaccurately or slowly. If the predicted relationship

    between the Thematic Hierarchy and Event Task performance is not found, that would suggest

    that the Thematic Hierarchy does not guide event roles in relation to PWA.

    8

    2.0 Methods

    2.1 Participants

    As reported in Dresang et al. (2019), Event Task data was collected from 208 participants,

    including 26 PWA and 182 neurologically healthy participants across the lifespan. These healthy

    participants were divided into groups of 15 in the following age ranges: 20-29, 30-39, 40-49, 50-

    59, 60-69, 70-79. The healthy control groups allowed for Event Task performance analysis across

    the lifespan. All healthy participants were required to have no history of language, speech, or

    neurological impairments. All participants attained a score of >24 on the Mini-Mental State

    Examination, an assessment of cognitive function (MMSE) (Folstein et al., 1975). All participants

    were monolingual native English speakers with normal or corrected-to-normal hearing and vision.

    All participants provided informed consent and received compensation for their participation in

    the study (Dresang et al., 2019). 4 participants were found to have incomplete Event Task data,

    resulting in exclusion from the analysis.

    2.2 Event Task Stimuli and Procedure

    The Event Task is composed of 260 colored images, including 256 experimental images

    and 4 practice images (Dresang et al., 2019). The photographs depict people engaging in basic

    actions in a variety of environments. Half of these images portray plausible events, such as a person

    boxing at a gym, and the other half portray implausible events, such as a man playing the violin

    9

    underwater. Implausible events are impossible or unlikely, and do not match our everyday

    experience or our world knowledge based in semantic memory. Implausible event images were

    characterized as stimuli that “violates so-called ‘world knowledge’ about typical human actions in

    ecological environments” (Proverbio & Riva, 2009). In each implausible image, at least one event

    role is incongruent with the event depicted. Incongruent event roles conflict with an individual’s

    semantic memory for typical events of this type. Participants were shown one image at a time and

    were asked to indicate if this scene was something that may normally happen by pressing a button

    on the keyboard—the key labeled “1” indicated that the event “might normally happen” (a

    plausible event) and the key labeled “5” indicated that the event “might not normally happen” (an

    implausible event). Participants were instructed to respond as accurately and as quickly as possible

    (Dresang et al., 2019). 

    2.3 Data Compilation

    I imported the Event Task data from E-Prime to Excel, and then consolidated the data into

    one Excel file. The final Event Task Dataset included information such as participant code, trial

    number, correct answer, accuracy, and reaction time. All participant demographic information was

    manually transferred from printed files into an Excel file, including date of birth, education, Mini-

    Mental State Examination score, and participant code. Each PWA was matched to a neurologically

    healthy participant with the same age and years of education. The dataset consisting of PWA and

    matched healthy participants was used to compare event role accuracy and reaction time between

    the two groups.

    10

    2.4 Event Taxonomy Questionnaire

    In order to evaluate which event role may have caused the incongruence in each

    implausible image, referred to as an event role violation, we used data from an Event Taxonomy

    Questionnaire. In the questionnaire, 37 volunteer participants were given event role definitions

    and were asked to indicate which event roles created the incongruency in the 130 implausible

    stimuli images from the Event Task. For example, in the image of a nail being hammered with a

    shoe, the implausible event role would be the instrument. 5 practice images were given with

    explanations as to which event role was violated to ensure comprehension of the instructions. The

    data consisted of 33 participants’ responses, as 4 participants were excluded due to incomplete

    questionnaires.

    The individual Event Taxonomy Questionnaire responses were compiled into one Excel

    file. The number of ratings for each event role dimension per stimuli image was totaled. The mean

    number of rated violations for each event role dimension across all incongruent stimuli was

    calculated. To be considered a canonical event role violation (for any given stimuli image), the

    number of ratings had to be 2 SD above the mean for that dimension. For example, if a stimuli

    image’s number of agent violation ratings was 2 SD above the mean number of agent violation

    ratings across all incongruent stimuli, that stimuli image would be classified as having a canonical

    agent violation. To also be considered an exclusive event role violation, the stimuli image must

    have canonical ratings for only one event role dimension. The data was then converted into binary

    data (“1” for an event role violation and “0” for lack thereof) which was imported to the Event

    Task Dataset. This data indicated which event role was violated per stimuli image per participant

    trial, allowing thematic hierarchy data analysis to occur.

    11

    2.5 Data Analysis

    The Event Taxonomy Questionnaire data was summarized via pivot table, presenting the

    number of canonical and exclusive event role violations in the incongruent stimuli set.

    Table 1: Canonical and Exclusive Event Role Violations

    Agent Patient Instrument Location

    Canonical Event Role Violations 25 23 18 29

    Exclusive Event Role Violations 16 19 13 23

    Exclusive event role violations were used to generate the ordered factor Thematic

    Hierarchy variable. Using exclusive violations ensured that stimuli images each have one clear

    event role violation, which made it possible to map that violation to a single position on the

    Thematic Hierarchy. Data analysis was conducted via R using a regression analysis including both

    linear mixed effects models and generalized linear mixed effects models.

    12

    3.0 Results

    3.1 PWA and Matched Controls Analyses

    An initial generalized linear mixed-effects model compared accuracy at detecting

    violations of four different event roles across PWA and healthy control participants who were

    matched to the PWA by age and years of education. All analyses were conducted using only

    incongruent stimuli. Model glmer(EventAccuracy ~ Group*ThemHierarchy + (1 +

    ThemHierarchy|Subject) + (1|Stimulus)) compared accuracy performance among event role

    violation types across both groups, while accounting for random effects across subjects and items.

    Agent violations were coded as 1 (i.e., the reference level) due to agent holding the highest position

    on the Thematic Hierarchy. Because agents are at the top of the Thematic Hierarchy, we expected

    them to yield the highest accuracy and lowest reaction time. Both groups’ average accuracy

    performance per event role violation type is depicted in Figure 1. Mean accuracy was relatively

    high for the agent and patient violations, at around 86%, and not much lower for the location

    violations. Instrument violations had the lowest accuracy. However, none of these differences were

    reliable (Agent vs. Instrument: Estimate = -0.75798, SE = 0.44675, p value = -0.0898), (Agent vs.

    Patient: Estimate = 0.01634, SE = 0.40747, p value = 0.9680), and (Agent vs. Location: Estimate

    = -0.21022, SE = 0.39464, p value = 0.5943). There was no reliable effect of Group (Estimate =

    0.24334, SE = 0.34004, p value = 0.4742) and no reliable interaction between Group and patient

    (Estimate = 0.14145, SE = 0.30828, p value = 0.6463), instrument (Estimate = 0.53204, SE =

    0.34735, p value = 0.1256), or location (Estimate = 0.28329, SE = 0.30130, p value = 0.3471).

    13

    Figure 1: PWA and matched control participants’ accuracy performance.

    A parallel analysis of reaction time included only trials on which participants answered

    correctly. We analyzed the time it took for participants to detect event role violations using the

    following model: lmer(RT ~ Group*ThemHierarchy + (1 + ThemHierarchy|Subject) +

    (1|Stimulus)). Figure 2 displays both groups’ average reaction time performance per event role

    violation type. Patient violations yielded the highest reaction times in both groups. In PWA, patient

    violation reaction times were about 200 to 300 ms slower than the average for agent, instrument,

    and location violations, which ranged from about 1950 ms to 2050 ms. In the healthy control group,

    patient violation reaction times were about 100 to 200 ms slower than the average for agent,

    instrument, and location violations, which ranged from about 1720 to 1820 ms. However, none of

    these differences were reliable (Agent vs. patient: Estimate = 126.01, SE = 104.99, t value =1.200),

    (Agent vs. instrument: Estimate = 29.25, SE = 114.58, t value = 0.255), and (Agent vs. location:

    Estimate = -172.57, SE = 100.63, t value = -1.715). Group had a reliable effect reaction time

    (Estimate = -388.502, SE = 158.553, t value = -2.450). No reliable interaction was found between

    0.6

    0.65

    0.7

    0.75

    0.8

    0.85

    0.9

    Agent Patient

    Instrument Location

    A
    cc

    ur
    ac

    y

    Event Role Violations

    PWA and Matched Healthy Controls: Accuracy

    PWA

    HC

    14

    Group and patient (Estimate = -34.384, SE = 74.984, t value = -0.459), instrument (Estimate =

    33.311, SE = 77.879, p value = 0.428), or location (Estimate = -8.582, SE = 69.416, p value =

    -0.124).

    Figure 2: PWA and matched control participants’

    reaction time performance.

    3.2 PWA Analyses

    The next set of analyses were identical to the ones reported in the previous section, except

    that they only included data from PWA. These analyses aim to investigate whether the Thematic

    Hierarchy guides PWA accuracy and reaction time performance on the Event Task, examining

    individual PWA performance in more depth than the previous analysis. Model

    glmer(EventAccuracy ~ ThemHierarchy + (1 + ThemHierarchy|Subject) + (1|Stimulus))

    compared accuracy performance among event role violation types in the PWA sample. As depicted

    50

    0

    700

    900

    1100

    1300

    1500

    1700

    1900

    2100

    2300

    2500

    Agent Patient Instrument Location

    R
    ea

    ct
    io

    n
    T

    im
    e

    (M
    S

    )

    Event Role Violations

    PWA and Matched Healthy Controls: Reaction Time

    PWA

    HC

    15

    in Figure 1, mean accuracy was approximately 85% for agent and patient violations with location

    violations following close behind, averaging 83%. Instrument violations had the lowest accuracy,

    averaging 76%. None of these differences were reliable (Agent vs Instrument: Estimate = –

    0.75384, SE = 0.40886, p value = 0.0652), (Agent vs. Patient: Estimate = 0.01364, SE = 0.37801,

    p value =0.9712), and (Agent vs. Location: Estimate = -0.16820, SE = 0.36538, p value = 0.6453).

    In contrast to the mean accuracy across all PWA per event role violation type reported in

    Figure 1, Figure 3 shows an in-depth description of accuracy performance per individual

    PWA.

    As depicted in Figure 3, 19 of 26 PWA displayed the lowest accuracy performance on instrument

    violations. This unexpected result was identified in the majority of PWA’s accuracy performance,

    which is particularly interesting considering the higher accuracy average of location violations,

    which are ranked lower on the Thematic Hierarchy than instruments and therefore were expected

    to display the lowest accuracy performance. One PWA displayed the expected pattern: agent

    violations being most accurate, followed by patient violations, instrument violations, and, finally,

    location violations. The six PWA who were furthest from the expected pattern displayed unique

    accuracy performance patterns which did not align with the expected pattern or common

    performance across all PWA. For example, participant 218 had the highest accuracy for instrument

    violations and the lowest accuracy for agent violations, which is far from both the expected pattern

    and the performance of most PWA in this sample. The PWA that were furthest from the expected

    pattern exemplify some of the variability seen across PWA accuracy performance.

    16

    Figure 3: Individual PWA accuracy performance per event role violation type.

    Model lmer(RT ~ ThemHierarchy + (1 + ThemHierarchy|Subject) + (1|Stimulus))

    compared reaction time performance among event role violation type in the PWA sample. Mean

    reaction time was relatively stable at around 2000 ms across agent, instrument, and location

    violations. Patient violations had the highest reaction time, averaging 2245 ms. However, none of

    these differences were reliable (Agent vs. Patient: Estimate = 198.706, SE = 123.046, t value =

    1.615), (Agent vs. Instrument: Estimate = -24.131, SE = 133.104, t value = -0.181), and (Agent

    vs. Location: Estimate = -33.977, SE = 115.503, t value = -0.294).

    In contrast to the mean reaction times for PWA per event role violation type provided in

    Figure 2, Figure 4 provides reaction time performance for individual PWA across all event role

    violation types. 18 of 26 PWA displayed the slowest reaction time for patient violations. None of

    the PWA exhibited the expected pattern of reaction time performance. One PWA was identified

    as being furthest from the expected pattern: participant 222 displayed the highest reaction time for

    agent violations, followed by instrument, patient, then location violations. This performance

    17

    pattern did not align with the expected pattern or common performance across all PWA. Although

    participant 222 exhibits a particularly unique pattern, reaction time variability is seen across all

    PWA.

    Figure 4: Individual PWA reaction time performance per event role violation type

    3.3 Lifespan Analyses

    The following analyses were conducted using Event Task data from 170 neurologically

    healthy adults across the lifespan, utilizing the incongruent Event Task trials to assess accuracy

    and reaction time in relation to the Thematic Hierarchy. These analyses investigate whether the

    Thematic Hierarchy guides Event Task performance on neurologically healthy adults across the

    lifespan. Additionally, these analyses include demographic information including age, MMSE

    score, and years of education to examine what other factors impact Event Task accuracy and

    reaction time performance.

    500

    1000

    1500

    2000

    2500

    3000

    3500

    4000

    4500

    201 202 203 204 205 206 207 208 209 210 211 212 213 214 215 216 217 218 219 220 221 222 223 224 225 227

    R
    ea

    ct
    io

    n
    T

    im
    e

    (M
    S

    )

    PWA

    PWA Individual Reaction Time Scores

    Agent

    Patient

    Instrument Location

    18

    3.3.1 Age Analyses

    A generalized linear mixed effects model investigated the relationship between accuracy,

    age, and the Thematic Hierarchy. The Thematic Hierarchy variable was coded with the R function

    as.integer, meaning that each event role violation type corresponds with an ascending number in

    accordance with the Thematic Hierarchy rankings. Agent, patient, instrument, and location

    violations are coded as 1, 2, 3, and 4 respectively. Model glmer(EventAccuracy ~

    Age*ThemHierarchy + (1 + ThemHierarchy|Subject) + (1|Stimulus)) indicated that age has a

    reliable effect on accuracy (Estimate = -0.015105, SE = 0.007156, p value = 0.0348). Figure 5

    provides average accuracy per event role violation type across the lifespan. As age increased,

    accuracy decreased on all event role violation types. The Thematic Hierarchy did not have a

    reliable effect on accuracy (Estimate = 0.028055, SE = 0.146167, p value = 0.8478). There was no

    reliable interaction found between age and Thematic Hierarchy (Estimate = -0.002082, SE =

    0.001851, p value = -0.2608).

    Figure 5: Healthy participants’ accuracy performance in relation to age.

    0.65

    0.7

    0.75

    0.8

    0.85

    0.9

    0.95

    1

    15 25 35 45 55 65 75 85

    A
    cc

    ur
    ac

    y

    Age in Years

    Neurologically Healthy Participants: Accuracy and Age

    Agent Patient Instrument Location

    Linear (Agent) Linear (Patient) Linear (Instrument) Linear (Location)

    19

    A linear mixed effects model assessed age and the Thematic Hierarchy in relation to

    reaction time. Model lmer((RT ~ Age*ThemHierarchy + (1 + ThemHierarchy|Subject) +

    (1|Stimulus)) indicated that age had a reliable effect on reaction time (Estimate = 7.161, SE =

    1.932, t value = 3.707). Figure 6 displays average reaction time performance per event role

    violation type across the lifespan. As age increases, reaction time increases. Thematic Hierarchy

    did not have a reliable effect on reaction time (Estimate = 29.83, SE = 29.09, t value = 1.026).

    There was no reliable interaction found between age and Thematic Hierarchy (Estimate = 0.013,

    SE = 0.2705, t value = 0.048).

    Figure 6: Healthy participants’ reaction time performance in relation to age.

    500

    1000

    1500

    2000

    2500

    3000

    3500

    4000

    15 25 35 45 55 65 75 85

    R
    ea

    ct
    io

    n
    T

    im
    e

    (M
    S

    )

    Age in Years

    Neurologically Healthy Participants: Reaction Time and Age

    Agent Patient Instrument Location

    Linear (Agent) Linear (Patient) Linear (Instrument) Linear (Location)

    20

    3.3.2 Mini-Mental State Examination Analyses

    Model glmer(EventAccuracy ~ MMSE*ThemHierarchy + (1 + ThemHierarchy|Subject) +

    (1|Stimulus)) assessed the relationship between MMSE Score and the Thematic Hierarchy in

    relation to accuracy. Figure 7 provides average accuracy performance per event role violation type

    in relation to MMSE score. MMSE Score did not have a reliable effect on accuracy performance

    (Estimate = -0.0001468, SE = 0.1550574, p value = 0.999). Similarly, the Thematic Hierarchy did

    not have a reliable effect on accuracy (Estimate = -1.9259424, SE = 1.2819478, p value = 0.133).

    There was not a reliable interaction found between MMSE score and the Thematic Hierarchy

    (Estimate = 0.0617778, SE = 0.0433683, p value = 0.154).

    Figure 7: Healthy participants’ accuracy performance in relation to MMSE Score.

    0.6

    0.65

    0.7

    0.75

    0.8

    0.85

    0.9

    0.95

    1

    25 26 27 28 29 30

    A
    cc

    ur
    ac

    y

    MMSE Score

    Neurologically Healthy Participants:
    Accuracy and MMSE Score

    Agent

    Patient

    Instrument

    Location

    21

    A linear mixed effects model examined MMSE score and Thematic Hierarchy in relation

    to reaction time. Model lmer(RT ~ MMSE*ThemHierarchy + (1 + ThemHierarchy|Subject) +

    (1|Stimulus)) revealed that participants with an MMSE score of 25 had higher reaction times than

    participants with MMSE scores of 26-28. Figure 8 provides average reaction time performance

    per event role violation type by MMSE score. MMSE score had a reliable effect on reaction time

    (Estimate = -88.520, SE = 43.632, t value = -2.029). Thematic Hierarchy did not have a reliable

    effect on reaction time (Estimate = -12.306, SE = 186.204, t value = -0.066). There was not a

    reliable interaction between MMSE and Thematic Hierarchy reaction time (Estimate = 1.543, SE

    = 6.245, t value = 0.247).

    Figure 8: Healthy participants’ reaction time performance in relation to MMSE Score.

    600

    800

    1000

    1200

    1400

    1600

    1800

    2000

    2200

    2400

    25 26 27 28 29 29.5 30

    R
    ea

    ct
    io

    n
    T

    im
    e

    (M
    S

    )

    MMSE Score

    Neurologically Healthy Participants:
    Reaction Time and MMSE Score

    Agent

    Patient

    Instrument

    Location

    22

    3.3.3 Years of Education Analyses

    A generalized linear mixed effects model examined years of education and Thematic

    Hierarchy in relation to accuracy. Figure 9 depicts average accuracy performance per event role

    violation type in relation to years of education. Model glmer(EventAccuracy ~

    YoE*ThemHierarchy + (1 + ThemHierarchy|Subject) + (1|Stimulus)) indicated that participants

    with 11 years of education were less accurate on all event role violation types than participants

    with 12-22 years of education. Neither years of education (Estimate = 0.023906, SE = 0.059237,

    p value = 0.68653) nor Thematic Hierarchy (Estimate = -0.009782, SE = 0.267561, p value =

    0.97083) had a reliable effect on accuracy. Additionally, there was no interaction between years

    of education and Thematic Hierarchy (Estimate = -0.006824, SE = 0.015047, p value = 0.65016).

    Figure 9: Healthy participants’ accuracy performance in relation to years of education.

    0

    0.2

    0.4

    0.6

    0.8

    1

    1.2

    11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22

    A
    cc

    ur
    ac

    y

    Years of Education

    Neurologically Healthy Participants:
    Accuracy and

    Years of Education

    Agent

    Patient

    Instrument

    Location

    23

    A linear mixed effects model analyzed how years of education and the Thematic Hierarchy

    predict reaction time. Figure 10 depicts average reaction time performance per event role violation

    type in relation to years of education. Model lmer(RT ~ YoE*ThemHierarchy + (1 +

    ThemHierarchy|Subject) + (1|Stimulus)) found that participants with 11 years of education had

    higher reaction times than participants with 12-22 years of education. However, years of education

    did not have a reliable effect on reaction time (Estimate = -3.1859, SE = 17.4183, t value = -0.183).

    The Thematic Hierarchy did not have a reliable effect on reaction time (Estimate = 42.8134, SE =

    46.1222, t value = 0.928). Years of education and Thematic Hierarchy did not have a reliable

    interaction (Estimate = -0.5739, SE = 2.3879, t value = -0.240).

    Figure 10: Healthy participants’ reaction time performance in relation to years of education.

    600

    1100

    1600

    2100

    2600

    3100

    11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22

    R
    ea

    ct
    io

    n
    T

    im
    e

    (M
    S

    )

    Years of Education

    Neurologically Healthy Participants:
    Reaction Time and Years of Education

    Agent

    Patient

    Instrument

    Location

    24

    4.0 Discussion

    This paper reports new analyses examining accuracy and reaction time performance of

    PWA and healthy control participants on incongruent trials of the Event Task. In the task,

    participants saw photographs of events that are either plausible or implausible and were asked to

    indicate if the scene depicted is something that may normally happen. Each incongruent stimuli

    image had at least one event role violation, and accuracy and reaction time performance were

    analyzed per event role violation type.

    I predicted violations that involved event roles from higher on the Thematic Hierarchy

    would be detected more accurately and more quickly. For example, agent violations were expected

    to be detected faster and more accurately, whereas location violations were expected to be detected

    less accurately and more slowly. This prediction was based on experimental evidence from the

    literature indicating that information about highly prominent event roles, such as agent and patient,

    is spontaneously encoded even when it is not directly relevant to the task (Hafri et al., 2018). In

    contrast, event roles at the bottom of the Thematic Hierarchy receive less attention: common

    instruments are often omitted from story retellings (Lockridge and Brennan, 2002) and locations

    were the only event role not primed in a single-word priming task (Ferretti et al., 2001).

    25

    4.1 PWA and Matched Controls

    Analyses of the current dataset did not yield any reliable differences between PWA and

    healthy controls; however, an interesting numerical pattern of responses was revealed. Both PWA

    and the healthy control participants displayed the same pattern: patient violations yielded the

    highest accuracy, followed by agents, locations, and instruments. This is inconsistent with the

    original hypothesis that agent violations would be most accurate, followed by patient violations,

    instrument violations, and location violations. On average across both groups, instrument

    violations were less accurate than other event role violations. In the healthy control group, patient,

    agent, and location violations were on average between 87% to 88% accurate while instrument

    violations averaged only 82% accuracy. Similarly, PWA were approximately 83% to 85% accurate

    on agent, patient, and location violations, while instrument violations were only 76% accurate on

    average. The majority of PWA were the least accurate in identifying instrument violations in

    comparison to other event role violation types on the Event Task incongruent trials.

    Both PWA and matched healthy participants reacted fastest to location violations, followed

    by instrument violations, then agent violations, and finally, patient violations. This finding is

    inconsistent with the hypothesis that both groups would have the lowest reaction time for agent

    violations, followed by patient violations, instrument violations, then locations violations. These

    findings indicate that the Thematic Hierarchy does not guide either group’s accuracy or reaction

    time performance on the Event Task. This finding leads to yet another question: if not the Thematic

    Hierarchy, what is guiding their performance?

    Both groups displayed the same pattern of accuracy and reaction time performance, most

    notably the low instrument event role violation accuracy. One possibility is that the size of the

    event role in the stimuli image affects accuracy and reaction time performance on the Event Task.

    26

    Instruments are often smaller than other event roles, such as agents and locations. Perhaps event

    roles which take up more pixels of the image, like locations, are easier to identify accurately and

    quickly in comparison to event roles which are often smaller and take up fewer pixels, such as

    instruments.

    This finding of relatively poor detection of atypical instruments seems to potentially

    contrast with Lockridge and Brennan’s (2002) finding that atypical instruments were often

    explicitly mentioned early in story retellings, whereas typical instruments were often omitted,

    suggesting that incongruent or atypical instruments receive more attention than inferable

    instruments. . However, their study compared typical instruments to atypical instruments, focusing

    solely on one event role category; in the current study, atypical instruments were compared to

    atypical agents, patients, and locations. While the Lockridge and Brennan experiment may speak

    to the prominence of incongruent instruments in comparison to common instruments, our study

    found that instrument violation accuracy was lower than all other event role violation types,

    indicating that instruments are not as prominent as agents, patients, or locations.

    Additionally, Lockridge and Brennan (2002) examined instrument prominence in story

    retelling, an act reliant on language comprehension and production. Participants were told the story

    and then asked to retell it, allowing ample time for story comprehension and the language

    production necessary for retelling. However, when making plausibility judgements in the Event

    Task, the participant relies on semantic memory rather than language processing. It might be the

    case that atypical instruments are prominent enough to receive more attention in language

    processing, aligning with their position above locations on the Thematic Hierarchy, but perhaps

    are not prominent enough for quick encoding of information during split-second judgements, such

    as the plausibility judgement in the Event Task.

    27

    4.2 Neurologically Healthy Adults Across the Lifespan

    Across the neurologically healthy adult lifespan analyses, Thematic Hierarchy did not have

    a significant effect on accuracy or reaction time. This finding is similar to the findings for PWA

    and matched controls in showing that the Thematic Hierarchy does not guide Event Task

    performance on incongruent stimuli items. Age had a significant effect on accuracy in

    neurologically healthy adults; increased age was correlated with lower accuracy on the Event Task.

    Additionally, age had a significant effect on reaction time, indicating that as an individual increases

    in age, their reaction time gradually slows.

    One potential mechanism behind this age-related decline in performance on the Event Task

    may be inefficiencies in older adults’ semantic networks. Semantic networks are the structural

    organization of mental representations which portray semantic relations in a fashion similar to that

    of a map. An efficient semantic network would be flexible with strong connectivity and little space

    between concepts on the map. A study conducted by Cosgrove et al. (2021) examined the effects

    of aging on semantic networks. They predicted that older adults would display semantic networks

    with less flexibility, indicating weak connectivity and larger distance between nodes. In their

    study, neurologically healthy older and younger adults participate in a categorical verbal fluency

    task. This task measures the ability to recall semantic knowledge from long-term memory, and

    results from this type of task are often utilized for mapping semantic networks. This data was

    examined using a percolation analysis to create visualized semantic networks. Findings indicated

    that older adults’ semantic networks were less efficient than those of younger adults (Cosgrove et

    al., 2021).

    In theory, older adults’ inefficient semantic networks may hinder performance on a variety

    of semantic memory tasks, including plausibility judgements such as the Event Task trials. I

    28

    believe that an efficient semantic network would lead to accurate, fast responses. For example, a

    younger person with efficient semantic networks may see the Event Task stimulus image of a man

    playing the violin underwater and quickly identify the incongruent location. Some concepts in a

    semantic network for the violinist could be a concert hall, orchestra, cello, or musician. However,

    the concepts of ocean and violinist would not be close to each other on a semantic network,

    informing the participant that the location must be incongruent. I predict that an older individual

    with an inefficient semantic network could take longer to conclude that the stimulus is incongruent

    because their semantic network has more distance between concepts, making the judgement a

    longer and more strenuous process. I think it is possible that as age increases, performance on

    semantic plausibility judgements worsens due to inefficient semantic networks.

    I also investigated potential relationships between performance on the Mini State Mental

    Exam (MMSE), an assessment of cognitive function (Folstein et al., 1975), and performance on

    detecting violations in the Event Task. The MMSE assesses skills relevant to orientation (i.e.,

    knowledge of what year it is), attention, recall, and language. It seemed likely that a person with a

    lower MMSE score might struggle to attend to the Event Task trials, resulting in low accuracy or

    high reaction time. In my analyses, MMSE score was not related to accuracy or reaction time

    performance. However, participants with a MMSE score of 25 had an average reaction time of

    2097 MS. Participants with a MMSE score ranging from 26-30 had an average reaction time of

    1570 MS. In summary, participants with a MMSE score of 25 were approximately 527 MS slower

    to respond than participants with a MMSE score ranging from 26-30. MMSE scores classified as

    “normal” range from 25-30 (Folstein et al., 1975). The slower reaction time of participants with a

    25 MMSE score could be indicative of their lower cognitive function, as their score is at the bottom

    of the normal range.

    29

    Years of education also did not have a significant effect on accuracy or reaction time

    performance. However, participants with 11 years of education were around 60% accurate for most

    event role violation types, while participants with 12-22 years of education were approximately

    90% accurate on most event role violation types. It seems that having 12 years of education

    increases accuracy performance by nearly 30% in comparison to those with 11 years of education.

    This finding indicates that having a high school degree is correlated with increased accuracy on

    the Event Task. Although years of education did not significantly affect reaction time, participants

    with only 11 years of education displayed remarkably higher reaction time performance.

    Participants with 11 years of education reacted in 2924 milliseconds (ms) on average. Participants

    with 12 years of education had a slightly faster reaction time of 1811 ms. Participants with 13-22

    years of education displayed a consistently faster reaction time around 1496 ms. Participants with

    11 years of education were approximately 1428 ms slower to respond to Event Task trials than

    participants with 13-22 years of education. This finding reinforces the interpretation that attaining

    a high school education level may improve performance on the Event Task in relation to both

    accuracy and reaction time.

    One potential explanation for this distinction between 11 years of education and 12+ years

    of education is that high school coursework may impact the richness of one’s semantic memory

    representations. High school students are exposed to a wide variety of coursework which can

    provide new insights about the world, adding to their ever-growing semantic memory. It’s

    plausible that high school classes introduce information which adds to their semantic memory and

    assists in making plausibility judgements. Completing a high school education may be critical in

    adding important general knowledge to one’s semantic memory during a person’s formative years.

    For example, exposure to broader subject matter through various courses could create more

    30

    efficient semantic networks featuring a wide range of concepts, leading to more accurate and faster

    responses to plausibility judgements such as the Event Task trials.

    Additionally, high school coursework is designed to improve critical thinking and

    analytical skills. High school assignments, such as analyzing passages in a literature class or

    solving complex open-response problems in math class, allow students to practice critical thinking

    on a daily basis, ultimately resulting in improved analytical skills. The Event Task plausibility task

    is similar to the theoretical questions high school students are often asked to answer in classes; a

    question is posed, and students use problem solving skills and general knowledge to come to the

    best possible answer. It’s possible that the completion of a high school education leads to improved

    analytical skills, resulting in better performance on the Event Task in comparison to individuals

    without a completed high school education.

    4.3 Clinical Application

    With this research comes the opportunity for future clinical advancements. Verb Network

    Strengthening Treatment (VNeST) is an aphasia treatment focused on ameliorating generalized

    word retrieval (both noun and verb retrieval) in PWA (Edmonds et al., 2014). I believe that

    implementing the Event Task into VNeST protocol could lead a better understanding of how

    aphasia treatment mitigates event knowledge deficits. Clinicians could utilize the Event Task

    before and after VNeST to evaluate potential accuracy and reaction time performance

    improvement, acting as a measure of VNeST’s success rates. Additionally, initial Event Task

    performance could help clinicians identify areas of difficulty (such as specific event role deficits)

    which could be focused on in treatment. Implementing event role analysis into VNeST could

    31

    provide useful insight into the ways that PWA process and store event role information, leading to

    novel clinical applications of VNeST. In the VNeST protocol, the clinician cues the PWA to

    generate agents and patients as a way of activating their semantic networks, followed by

    encouraging the expansion of schemas (i.e., where, when, and why is the agent doing that action?).

    PWA then attempt to independently produce a verb relevant to the event. Finally, the protocol is

    repeated with the omission of the clinician cues to promote independence. An additional step

    includes asking the PWA to produce a subject-verb-object sentence describing an event image.

    Future research could entail adding eye tracking to this step to examine the order in which

    PWA look at each event role and the duration of each glance. I would be curious to see if PWA

    would display a similar pattern of results as to those found by Griffin and Bock (2000). Their eye-

    tracking data indicated that neurologically healthy adults gazed at the agent before speaking, then

    glanced at the patient as they began speaking, and concluded by looking to the agent once again.

    Adding an eye tracking step to VNeST treatment would allow us to examine how/if the order of

    event role gazing or the duration of gaze changes over the course of treatment. This would expand

    upon the previous experiment by Griffin and Bock (2000) by tracking PWAs’ gazing patterns in

    relation to all four event role types, whereas the previous study only examined neurologically

    healthy adults’ eye movements for simplistic images featuring only agents and patients. I would

    predict that, in the beginning phases of treatment, PWA may look between the event roles multiple

    times prior to speech, spending more time gazing at any event role with which they are struggling

    with word retrieval. I think that PWA would spend the most time gazing at agents and patients.

    Additionally, it’s possible that PWA may spend less time glancing at instruments, as the current

    study’s results indicate that instruments may not be particularly salient. However, I would expect

    32

    that over the course of treatment, PWA may begin to show the same pattern as the neurologically

    healthy participants in the Griffin and Bock (2000) experiment.

    Another future direction could entail examining what factors influence generalized lexical

    retrieval, specifically in relation to event roles and semantic relations. Semantic memory can be

    divided into two distinct sectors: taxonomic and thematic knowledge. Taxonomic knowledge

    consists of the relation between two entities founded in similarities such as common features (i.e.,

    dog and cat), whereas thematic knowledge refers to the relationship between two entities which

    commonly occur in the same events (i.e., dog and leash) (Mirman et al., 2017). Taxonomic

    organization would separate entities by categories (such as event roles) while thematic

    organization would distinguish those same entities by events in which they often co-occur (Mirman

    et al., 2017).

    I would expect that PWA would exhibit improved generalized lexical retrieval in response

    to untrained words that are thematically related to trained words. For example, I predict that a

    trained word such as “dog” might elicit generalized retrieval for untrained words such as “leash”

    or “fur” which are thematically related. I hypothesize that generalized lexical retrieval may rely

    heavily on the richness of one’s semantic networks. An efficient, dense semantic network would

    have less distance between related concepts, which I predict would result in a higher success rate

    of generalized word retrieval in PWA.

    The distinction between thematic and taxonomic relations arises yet another question:

    could lexical retrieval improvement of trained words result in generalized lexical retrieval across

    untrained words of the same event role category? For example, perhaps a person trained on the

    word “hammer” could easily retrieve the untrained word “baseball bat,” not because they are

    thematically related, but because they fall into the same taxonomic category of “instrument” and

    33

    share the purpose of helping the agent to complete an action. I hypothesize that the success of

    generalized lexical retrieval across event role types would be dependent on the strength of the

    participant’s semantic networks, more specifically the distance between concepts which are

    taxonomically related instead of thematically related. Efficient, taxonomically organized semantic

    networks could result in generalized lexical retrieval across event role types.

    Future research could examine semantic network efficiency and organization to gain a

    better understanding of how semantic relations are represented in the mind, answering questions

    such as: are there taxonomic semantic networks solely detailing the categorization of entities into

    event roles? Opposingly, do semantic networks display both thematic and taxonomic relations on

    the same semantic network? If so, I would expect that thematically related concepts would be

    closer together on the map, whereas taxonomically related concepts would have more distance

    between nodes. These questions can be addressed in future research delving into how event

    representations are stored in an individual’s semantic memory.

    To summarize, examining the gazing patterns of PWA during VNeST treatment could

    provide useful insight into the ways that PWA process event role information before and during

    speech. Considering taxonomic relations, thematic relations, and semantic network efficiency

    could lead to novel findings describing the mechanisms behind generalized lexical retrieval in

    PWA. In the future, the semantic memory research conducted in relation to the Thematic Hierarchy

    and event knowledge could lead to the implementation of new and improved treatment methods

    for those PWA.

    34

    4.4 Limitations

    A limitation of this research is the variation across stimuli images. The Event Task dataset

    consists of 256 stimuli images, including both congruent and incongruent stimuli. Among the 128

    incongruent stimuli, there are examples of each event role violation type; however, the number of

    stimuli portraying each event role violation type varies. For example, there were 23 location-

    exclusive violation stimuli, whereas there were only 13 instrument-exclusive violation stimuli.

    This severely limited the power of my analyses to detect effects. Additionally, the stimuli images

    vary in how many event role violations are present. Some Event Task stimuli images include

    multiple event role violations—for example, a single stimuli image may have both agent and

    patient violations. Future studies could include a new stimuli set which would ensure that all event

    role violation types are featured equally across stimuli images and each stimuli image contains

    only one event role violation. Creating a specialized stimuli set featuring carefully selected event

    role violations could lead to statistically stronger results in future work.

    In addition to the variable number of items per event role violation type, the current study

    featured only a small number of items. The low number of incongruent stimuli items led to a lack

    of statistical power in our analyses, resulting in reduced ability to detect meaningful differences

    between event role violation types. Creating a new stimuli set including equal and larger numbers

    of items per event role violation type would help to address this limitation in future work.

    35

    5.0 Conclusion

    The current study examined accuracy and reaction time performance on the Event Task in

    relation to the Thematic Hierarchy. In summary, the Thematic Hierarchy does not seem to guide

    Event Task performance in PWA or neurologically healthy adults across the lifespan. However,

    PWA and neurologically healthy controls alike displayed the same patterns of both accuracy and

    reaction time performance, indicating that an unknown force may be guiding their performance.

    Most notably, participants displayed low accuracy performance on instrument violation

    incongruent trials. This finding could be driven by event role size or lack of instrument prominence

    when viewing the Event Task stimuli.

    Although no results were found to be statistically reliable, interesting numerical patterns

    emerged from the current study. In neurologically healthy adults, age had a reliable effect on

    accuracy and reaction time performance, which may be influenced by the deterioration of semantic

    networks associated with aging (Cosgrove et al., 2021). Additionally, neurologically healthy adults

    with 12+ years of education were found to have better accuracy and reaction time performance

    than adults with only 11 years of education, which may be indicative of how high school education

    completion may impact critical thinking skills and semantic memory richness.

    The current study provided novel findings which may be the foundation for future research.

    Next steps would include creating an improved version of the Event Task featuring stimuli images

    specifically designed to assess performance on each event role type. Future studies could examine

    VNeST in relation to the event roles with the hopes of getting a better understanding of how PWA

    process event roles while simultaneously improving treatment methods. Finally, next steps could

    include studying the role of semantic networks in regard to aphasia treatment and generalized

    36

    lexical retrieval. Future research examining PWA and event knowledge is imperative for both the

    scientific community and the clinical population.

    37

    Bibliography

    Antonucci, S., & Reilly, J. (2008). Semantic Memory and Language Processing: A Primer.
    Seminars in Speech and Language, 29(1), 005–017. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2008-
    1061621

    Bak, T. H., & Hodges, J. R. (2003). Kissing and dancing—A test to distinguish the lexical and
    conceptual contributions to noun/verb and action/object dissociation. Preliminary results
    in patients with frontotemporal dementia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 16(2), 169–181.
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0911-6044(02)00011-8

    Baker, M.C. (1997). Thematic roles and syntactic structure. In L. Haegeman (Ed.) Handbook of
    generative syntax (pp. 73–137). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Bozeat, S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Patterson, K., Garrard, P., & Hodges, J. R. (2000). Non-verbal
    semantic impairment in semantic dementia. Neuropsychologia, 38(9), 1207–1215.
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00034-8

    Caramazza, A., & Zurif, E. B. (1976). Dissociation of algorithmic and heuristic processes in
    language comprehension: Evidence from aphasia. Brain and Language, 3(4), 572–582.
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934x(76)90048-1

    Cosgrove, A. L., Kenett, Y. N., Beaty, R. E., & Diaz, M. T. (2021). Quantifying flexibility in
    thought: The resiliency of semantic networks differs across the lifespan. Cognition, 211,
    104631. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104631

    Dresang, H. C., Dickey, M. W., & Warren, T. C. (2019). Semantic memory for objects, actions,
    and events: A novel test of event-related conceptual semantic knowledge. Cognitive
    Neuropsychology, 36(7–8), 313–335. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643294.2019.1656604

    Edmonds, L., Mammino, K., & Ojeda, J. (2014). Effect of Verb Network Strengthening Treatment
    (VNeST) in Persons With Aphasia: Extension and Replication of Previous Findings.
    American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology / American Speech-Language-Hearing
    Association, 23. https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_AJSLP-13-0098

    Ferretti, T. R., McRae, K., & Hatherell, A. (2001). Integrating Verbs, Situation Schemas, and
    Thematic Role Concepts. Journal of Memory and Language, 44(4), 516–547.
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2728

    Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: A practical method
    for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research,
    12(3), 189–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

    38

    Griffin, Z. M., & Bock, K. (2000). What the Eyes Say about Speaking. Psychological Science,
    11(4), 274–279. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00255

    Hafri, A., Trueswell, J. C., & Strickland, B. (2018). Encoding of event roles from visual scenes is
    rapid, spontaneous, and interacts with higher-level visual processing. Cognition, 175, 36–
    52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.02.011

    Howard, D., Patterson, K., & Thames Valley Test Company. (1992). The pyramids and palm trees
    test: A test of semantic access from words and pictures. Thames Valley Test Company.

    Jackendoff, R. (1990). Semantic structures. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Lockridge, C. B., & Brennan, S. E. (2002). Addressees’ needs influence speakers’ early syntactic
    choices. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(3), 550–557.
    https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03196312

    McRae, K., & Matsuki, K. (2009). People Use their Knowledge of Common Events to Understand
    Language, and Do So as Quickly as Possible. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3(6),
    1417–1429. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2009.00174.x

    Mirman, D., Landrigan, J.-F., & Britt, A. E. (2017). Taxonomic and thematic semantic systems.
    Psychological Bulletin, 143(5), 499–520. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000092

    Proverbio, A. M., & Riva, F. (2009). RP and N400 ERP components reflect semantic violations in
    visual processing of human actions. Neuroscience Letters, 459(3), 142–146.
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2009.05.012

    Ünal, E., Richards, C., Trueswell, J. C., & Papafragou, A. (2021). Representing agents, patients,
    goals and instruments in causative events: A cross-linguistic investigation of early
    language and cognition. Developmental Science, 24(6), e13116.
    https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13116

      Title Page

    • Committee Page
    • Abstract

      Table of Contents

      Preface

      List of Tables

      List of Figures

      Preface

      1.0 Introduction

      1.1 Semantic memory and event knowledge

      1.2 The Thematic Hierarchy

      1.3 The Event Task

      1.4 The Current Study

      2.0 Methods

      2.1 Participants

      2.2 Event Task Stimuli and Procedure

      2.3 Data Compilation

      2.4 Event Taxonomy Questionnaire

      2.5 Data Analysis

      3.0 Results

      3.1 PWA and Matched Controls Analyses

      3.2 PWA Analyses

      3.3 Lifespan Analyses

      3.3.1 Age Analyses

      3.3.2 Mini-Mental State Examination Analyses

      3.3.3 Years of Education Analyses

      4.0 Discussion

      4.1 PWA and Matched Controls

      4.2 Neurologically Healthy Adults Across the Lifespan

      4.3 Clinical Application

      4.4 Limitations

      5.0 Conclusion

      Bibliography

    Expert paper writers are just a few clicks away

    Place an order in 3 easy steps. Takes less than 5 mins.

    Calculate the price of your order

    You will get a personal manager and a discount.
    We'll send you the first draft for approval by at
    Total price:
    $0.00